The survival of predators and prey depends on
their respective abilities to successfully chase food and escape
capture, thereby exerting strong selective pressure on their running
ability and behavioural strategies. Perhaps nowhere on Earth does this
play out more dramatically than on the African savannah, where the
fastest terrestrial predators chase their fleet-footed prey.
Yet direct
measures of the key factors driving this type of hunt performance in the
wild are difficult to obtain. In a paper published in Nature, Wilson et al.1
report findings from their use of data-capturing collars to track the
movement dynamics of wild animals in Botswana during hunts. The authors
also conducted computer modelling of predator–prey interactions and
carried out laboratory tests to assess the properties of the animals’
muscles.
In recent years, the ability to use remote-sensing devices
under natural field conditions and over long time frames has led many to
study animals’ migratory2,3, foraging4 and collective-movement behaviour5,6,
which has provided fascinating insights into biomechanics, physiology
and decision-making. Wilson and colleagues took a remote-sensing
approach to study lions preying on zebras, and cheetahs preying on
impala, in the wild. The authors temporarily immobilized animals and
fitted them with lightweight collars containing technically
sophisticated, custom-designed, miniature electronic and Global
Positioning System (GPS) devices. The devices monitored the animals’
location, movement direction and acceleration patterns. Wilson et al. tracked
9 lions, 5 cheetahs, 7 zebras and 7 impala, and recorded 2,726
high-speed runs for lions, 520 for cheetahs, 1,801 for zebras and 515
for impala. This remarkable data set logs individual animal strides and
provides information about the speed, acceleration and turning
performance of these predator–prey pairs.
The animals were not
observed directly, and one limitation of the recorded data is that few,
if any, of the movement tracks represented hunts between pairs of
predator and prey, with both animals recorded as one hunts the other.
Therefore, the hunting strategies of predator and prey must be inferred
from the collar-recorded data, making the assumption that the movement
patterns represent actual hunts. However, the locomotor performance
recorded by the remote-sensing collars and the hunting strategies that
could be inferred from these measurements are consistent with
behavioural observations made by others7.
Moreover, analysis of the full data set revealed that predators and
prey exhibited manoeuvrability near the limits of their capability.
Hence, although recordings of one-on-one hunts are lacking, the data
were consistent with maximal predator-pursuit and prey-evasion
performance, enabling the authors to model hunt outcomes.
After collar placement, a tiny biopsy of hindlimb muscle was
taken from the animals for subsequent state-of-the-art laboratory
testing of single-muscle-fibre contractility. This revealed that,
compared with the muscle fibres sampled from the prey species, the
predator muscle fibres deliver more power for a given muscle mass when
they contract, allowing the predators to run faster and accelerate and
decelerate more quickly than their prey. With more-powerful muscles than
their prey and claws to grip the ground effectively, predators are
better at accelerating into a turn (centripetal acceleration) than their
prey are.
Wilson and colleagues’ acceleration and GPS recordings
indicated that, during inferred hunts, the predators and prey regularly
achieved their maximal turning performance but ran at speeds well below
their athletic capabilities.
Running at speeds slower than maximum
capacity during a pursuit enhances manoeuvrability, which improves the
prey’s probability of successful escape and enables predators to better
track their prey’s movements, thereby increasing the number of
successful hunts.
Using their field-recorded locomotion data,
Wilson and colleagues modelled predator and prey capture–evasion tactics
to examine how different performance metrics, such as speed, separation
distance between the animals, deceleration, acceleration and turning
rate, would affect the outcome of a hunt. Evasion modelling showed that
prey escape was more likely if a prey animal relied on turning more
sharply and at a greater rate than its pursuer.
This type of behaviour
increases the unpredictability of the prey’s movement trajectory, as has
also been observed for bipedal desert rodents fleeing a predator8. Wilson et al. noted
that, during the predators’ approach (Fig. 1), they exhibited greater
deceleration and acceleration than that of the prey, allowing the
predators to close in on and better track the prey’s lateral movements.
The close match of athletic performance between predators and prey
highlights the strong selection pressure that has resulted in an
evolutionary ‘arms race’ for improved locomotion ability in large
carnivores and their large herbivorous prey.
Figure 1 | A lioness hunting a zebra in Etosha National Park, Namibia. Wilson et al.1
report their analysis of the movement dynamics of predator–prey hunts
in the wild in Africa using data gathered remotely from Global
Positioning System sensing collars placed on lions, zebras, cheetahs and
impala. Credit: Getty
The increasing use of remote-sensing technologies in animal
studies is enabling the monitoring of factors such as animal
acceleration, pressure (for example, during flight or when swimming at
depth) and temperature. Such work promises to illuminate not only
predator–prey interactions, but also how wild animals cope with other
real-world issues9,10.
For example, this type of research could enhance our understanding of
how animals are dealing with the impacts of climate change, or offer
insight into the factors governing behaviours such as habitat selection,
mating and foraging. Moreover, understanding how animals move might
inspire the design of robots that can negotiate complex environments.
Nature554, 176-178 (2018)
doi: 10.1038/d41586-018-01278-w
Nenhum comentário:
Postar um comentário
Observação: somente um membro deste blog pode postar um comentário.
Nenhum comentário:
Postar um comentário
Observação: somente um membro deste blog pode postar um comentário.